Comparison of types of generalizations and problem-solving schemas used to solve a mathematical problem

A cognitive schema is a mechanism which allows an individual to organize her/his experiences in such a way that a new similar experience can easily be recognised and dealt with successfully. Well-structured schemas provide for the knowledge base for subsequent mathematical activities. A new experien...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Educational studies in mathematics 2015-06, Vol.89 (2), p.283-306
Hauptverfasser: Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik, Kolar, Vida Manfreda
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 306
container_issue 2
container_start_page 283
container_title Educational studies in mathematics
container_volume 89
creator Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik
Kolar, Vida Manfreda
description A cognitive schema is a mechanism which allows an individual to organize her/his experiences in such a way that a new similar experience can easily be recognised and dealt with successfully. Well-structured schemas provide for the knowledge base for subsequent mathematical activities. A new experience can be assimilated into a previously existing schema or, if that is not possible, an existing schema must be accommodated to a new experience. These processes are applied in all the different areas of mathematics, including mathematical problem solving. The paper presents a study conducted with primary teacher students, the aim of which was to gain a deeper insight into the way of reasoning of individuals when presented with a mathematical problem. The types of generalization used by individuals and their problem solving schemas were analysed. The research results lead to some interesting conclusions. The type of generalization used for solving a problem is related to the type of the problem: if the relationship between the problem and the mathematical concept can be established, the reasoning type of generalization prevails. The results also confirm the importance of a well-structured schema for the successful solving of a complex mathematical problem. The students applying unstructured or partially formed schémas had problems when addressing a complex problem, whereas the students who were able to solve it mostly accommodated or assimilated their knowledge, which again proves these processes to be a necessary prerequisite for successful problem solving.
doi_str_mv 10.1007/s10649-015-9598-y
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_gale_infotracmisc_A433844431</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A433844431</galeid><ericid>EJ1059331</ericid><jstor_id>43590254</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>A433844431</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c394t-c4b74548997f262f11b37cf519ebe976e090178d81b13f62aae055ec887a5e793</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkEFvFCEYhonRxLX1B3gwIfHkgQoDDHBsNq3WNDGp7Zkw7DcjmxnYAGu6_npnHG3Smye-8D7ve3gQesfoBaNUfSqMtsIQyiQx0mhyeoE2TCpOqGbtS7ShlHHCjBSv0ZtS9pRSPdc2aNim6eByKCni1ON6OkBZjgEiZDeGX66GFAt2cYcPOXUjTKSk8WeIAy7-B0yu4GOBHa4JL_-AHZ5cXYIavBv_lc7Rq96NBd7-fc_Qw_XV_fYLuf32-WZ7eUs8N6ISLzolpNDGqL5pm56xjivfS2agA6NaoIYypXeadYz3beMcUCnBa62cBGX4Gfqw7g5uBBtin2p2fgrF20vBuRZCcDZTH59RPsUKj3Vwx1Lszfe75yxbWZ9TKRl6e8hhcvlkGbWLe7u6t7N7u7i3p7nzfu1ADv6Jv_rKqDT8z2az5mXO4gDZ7tMxx1nM_4zuS035aVVwaWgjBf8NqFmaSw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>Comparison of types of generalizations and problem-solving schemas used to solve a mathematical problem</title><source>Jstor Complete Legacy</source><source>Springer Nature - Complete Springer Journals</source><source>EBSCOhost Education Source</source><source>JSTOR Mathematics &amp; Statistics</source><creator>Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik ; Kolar, Vida Manfreda</creator><creatorcontrib>Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik ; Kolar, Vida Manfreda</creatorcontrib><description>A cognitive schema is a mechanism which allows an individual to organize her/his experiences in such a way that a new similar experience can easily be recognised and dealt with successfully. Well-structured schemas provide for the knowledge base for subsequent mathematical activities. A new experience can be assimilated into a previously existing schema or, if that is not possible, an existing schema must be accommodated to a new experience. These processes are applied in all the different areas of mathematics, including mathematical problem solving. The paper presents a study conducted with primary teacher students, the aim of which was to gain a deeper insight into the way of reasoning of individuals when presented with a mathematical problem. The types of generalization used by individuals and their problem solving schemas were analysed. The research results lead to some interesting conclusions. The type of generalization used for solving a problem is related to the type of the problem: if the relationship between the problem and the mathematical concept can be established, the reasoning type of generalization prevails. The results also confirm the importance of a well-structured schema for the successful solving of a complex mathematical problem. The students applying unstructured or partially formed schémas had problems when addressing a complex problem, whereas the students who were able to solve it mostly accommodated or assimilated their knowledge, which again proves these processes to be a necessary prerequisite for successful problem solving.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0013-1954</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1573-0816</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s10649-015-9598-y</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Dordrecht: Springer</publisher><subject>Analysis ; Comparative Analysis ; Education ; Elementary Education ; Generalization ; Logical Thinking ; Mathematics ; Mathematics Education ; Mathematics problems ; Preservice Teachers ; Problem Solving ; Schemas (Psychology) ; Schemata (Cognition)</subject><ispartof>Educational studies in mathematics, 2015-06, Vol.89 (2), p.283-306</ispartof><rights>Springer Science+Business Media 2015</rights><rights>Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2015</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2015 Springer</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c394t-c4b74548997f262f11b37cf519ebe976e090178d81b13f62aae055ec887a5e793</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c394t-c4b74548997f262f11b37cf519ebe976e090178d81b13f62aae055ec887a5e793</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/43590254$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/43590254$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,799,828,27901,27902,41464,42533,51294,57992,57996,58225,58229</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1059331$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kolar, Vida Manfreda</creatorcontrib><title>Comparison of types of generalizations and problem-solving schemas used to solve a mathematical problem</title><title>Educational studies in mathematics</title><addtitle>Educ Stud Math</addtitle><description>A cognitive schema is a mechanism which allows an individual to organize her/his experiences in such a way that a new similar experience can easily be recognised and dealt with successfully. Well-structured schemas provide for the knowledge base for subsequent mathematical activities. A new experience can be assimilated into a previously existing schema or, if that is not possible, an existing schema must be accommodated to a new experience. These processes are applied in all the different areas of mathematics, including mathematical problem solving. The paper presents a study conducted with primary teacher students, the aim of which was to gain a deeper insight into the way of reasoning of individuals when presented with a mathematical problem. The types of generalization used by individuals and their problem solving schemas were analysed. The research results lead to some interesting conclusions. The type of generalization used for solving a problem is related to the type of the problem: if the relationship between the problem and the mathematical concept can be established, the reasoning type of generalization prevails. The results also confirm the importance of a well-structured schema for the successful solving of a complex mathematical problem. The students applying unstructured or partially formed schémas had problems when addressing a complex problem, whereas the students who were able to solve it mostly accommodated or assimilated their knowledge, which again proves these processes to be a necessary prerequisite for successful problem solving.</description><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Comparative Analysis</subject><subject>Education</subject><subject>Elementary Education</subject><subject>Generalization</subject><subject>Logical Thinking</subject><subject>Mathematics</subject><subject>Mathematics Education</subject><subject>Mathematics problems</subject><subject>Preservice Teachers</subject><subject>Problem Solving</subject><subject>Schemas (Psychology)</subject><subject>Schemata (Cognition)</subject><issn>0013-1954</issn><issn>1573-0816</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2015</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqNkEFvFCEYhonRxLX1B3gwIfHkgQoDDHBsNq3WNDGp7Zkw7DcjmxnYAGu6_npnHG3Smye-8D7ve3gQesfoBaNUfSqMtsIQyiQx0mhyeoE2TCpOqGbtS7ShlHHCjBSv0ZtS9pRSPdc2aNim6eByKCni1ON6OkBZjgEiZDeGX66GFAt2cYcPOXUjTKSk8WeIAy7-B0yu4GOBHa4JL_-AHZ5cXYIavBv_lc7Rq96NBd7-fc_Qw_XV_fYLuf32-WZ7eUs8N6ISLzolpNDGqL5pm56xjivfS2agA6NaoIYypXeadYz3beMcUCnBa62cBGX4Gfqw7g5uBBtin2p2fgrF20vBuRZCcDZTH59RPsUKj3Vwx1Lszfe75yxbWZ9TKRl6e8hhcvlkGbWLe7u6t7N7u7i3p7nzfu1ADv6Jv_rKqDT8z2az5mXO4gDZ7tMxx1nM_4zuS035aVVwaWgjBf8NqFmaSw</recordid><startdate>20150601</startdate><enddate>20150601</enddate><creator>Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik</creator><creator>Kolar, Vida Manfreda</creator><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Netherlands</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ISR</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20150601</creationdate><title>Comparison of types of generalizations and problem-solving schemas used to solve a mathematical problem</title><author>Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik ; Kolar, Vida Manfreda</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c394t-c4b74548997f262f11b37cf519ebe976e090178d81b13f62aae055ec887a5e793</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2015</creationdate><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Comparative Analysis</topic><topic>Education</topic><topic>Elementary Education</topic><topic>Generalization</topic><topic>Logical Thinking</topic><topic>Mathematics</topic><topic>Mathematics Education</topic><topic>Mathematics problems</topic><topic>Preservice Teachers</topic><topic>Problem Solving</topic><topic>Schemas (Psychology)</topic><topic>Schemata (Cognition)</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kolar, Vida Manfreda</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><jtitle>Educational studies in mathematics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Čadež, Tatjana Hodnik</au><au>Kolar, Vida Manfreda</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1059331</ericid><atitle>Comparison of types of generalizations and problem-solving schemas used to solve a mathematical problem</atitle><jtitle>Educational studies in mathematics</jtitle><stitle>Educ Stud Math</stitle><date>2015-06-01</date><risdate>2015</risdate><volume>89</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>283</spage><epage>306</epage><pages>283-306</pages><issn>0013-1954</issn><eissn>1573-0816</eissn><abstract>A cognitive schema is a mechanism which allows an individual to organize her/his experiences in such a way that a new similar experience can easily be recognised and dealt with successfully. Well-structured schemas provide for the knowledge base for subsequent mathematical activities. A new experience can be assimilated into a previously existing schema or, if that is not possible, an existing schema must be accommodated to a new experience. These processes are applied in all the different areas of mathematics, including mathematical problem solving. The paper presents a study conducted with primary teacher students, the aim of which was to gain a deeper insight into the way of reasoning of individuals when presented with a mathematical problem. The types of generalization used by individuals and their problem solving schemas were analysed. The research results lead to some interesting conclusions. The type of generalization used for solving a problem is related to the type of the problem: if the relationship between the problem and the mathematical concept can be established, the reasoning type of generalization prevails. The results also confirm the importance of a well-structured schema for the successful solving of a complex mathematical problem. The students applying unstructured or partially formed schémas had problems when addressing a complex problem, whereas the students who were able to solve it mostly accommodated or assimilated their knowledge, which again proves these processes to be a necessary prerequisite for successful problem solving.</abstract><cop>Dordrecht</cop><pub>Springer</pub><doi>10.1007/s10649-015-9598-y</doi><tpages>24</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0013-1954
ispartof Educational studies in mathematics, 2015-06, Vol.89 (2), p.283-306
issn 0013-1954
1573-0816
language eng
recordid cdi_gale_infotracmisc_A433844431
source Jstor Complete Legacy; Springer Nature - Complete Springer Journals; EBSCOhost Education Source; JSTOR Mathematics & Statistics
subjects Analysis
Comparative Analysis
Education
Elementary Education
Generalization
Logical Thinking
Mathematics
Mathematics Education
Mathematics problems
Preservice Teachers
Problem Solving
Schemas (Psychology)
Schemata (Cognition)
title Comparison of types of generalizations and problem-solving schemas used to solve a mathematical problem
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-14T21%3A17%3A40IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Comparison%20of%20types%20of%20generalizations%20and%20problem-solving%20schemas%20used%20to%20solve%20a%20mathematical%20problem&rft.jtitle=Educational%20studies%20in%20mathematics&rft.au=%C4%8Cade%C5%BE,%20Tatjana%20Hodnik&rft.date=2015-06-01&rft.volume=89&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=283&rft.epage=306&rft.pages=283-306&rft.issn=0013-1954&rft.eissn=1573-0816&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s10649-015-9598-y&rft_dat=%3Cgale_cross%3EA433844431%3C/gale_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_galeid=A433844431&rft_ericid=EJ1059331&rft_jstor_id=43590254&rfr_iscdi=true