Physical and cognitive doping in university students using the unrelated question model

Study objectives A short paper-and-pencil questionnaire was distributed to 1.243 university students assessing the 12-month prevalence of physical and cognitive doping using two versions of the UQM with different probabilities for receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almo...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:PloS one 2018-05, Vol.13 (5), p.e0197270
Hauptverfasser: Dietz, Pavel, Quermann, Anne, van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria, Striegel, Heiko, Schröter, Hannes, Ulrich, Rolf, Simon, Perikles
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page
container_issue 5
container_start_page e0197270
container_title PloS one
container_volume 13
creator Dietz, Pavel
Quermann, Anne
van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria
Striegel, Heiko
Schröter, Hannes
Ulrich, Rolf
Simon, Perikles
description Study objectives A short paper-and-pencil questionnaire was distributed to 1.243 university students assessing the 12-month prevalence of physical and cognitive doping using two versions of the UQM with different probabilities for receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3). Likelihood ratio tests were used to assess whether the prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping differed significantly between p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3. The order of questions (physical doping and cognitive doping) as well as the probability of receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 or p [almost equal to] 2/3) were counterbalanced across participants. Statistical power analyses were performed to determine sample size. The prevalence estimate for physical doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3 was 22.5% (95% CI: 10.8-34.1), and 12.8% (95% CI: 7.6-18.0) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. For cognitive doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3, the estimated prevalence was 22.5% (95% CI: 11.0-34.1), whereas it was 18.0% (95% CI: 12.5-23.5) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. Likelihood-ratio tests revealed that prevalence estimates for both physical and cognitive doping, respectively, did not differ significantly under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3 (physical doping: X.sup.2 = 2.25, df = 1, p = 0.13; cognitive doping: X.sup.2 = 0.49, df = 1, p = 0.48). Bayes factors computed with the Savage-Dickey method favored the null ("the prevalence estimates are identical under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") over the alternative ("the prevalence estimates differ under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") hypothesis for both physical doping (BF = 2.3) and cognitive doping (BF = 5.3). The present results suggest that prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping assessed by the UQM are largely unaffected by the probability for receiving the sensitive question p.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0197270
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_gale_incontextgauss_ISR_A538804334</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A538804334</galeid><sourcerecordid>A538804334</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-g994-d08afe13a807d1ad3951650018e559a844aef00ca3e0d389e933e8d427ed68bd3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFj01LAzEYhIMoWKv_wENOgoddk81-JMdS_CgUKlr0WF43b3dTYlKbROy_t0UP9eRpZpiHgSHkkrOci4bfrHzaOLD52jvMGVdN0bAjMuBKFFldMHF84E_JWQgrxioh63pAXh_7bTAtWApO09Z3zkTziVT7tXEdNY4mt8ubYOKWhpg0uhhoCvsy9rhrN2ghoqYfCUM03tF3r9Gek5Ml2IAXvzok87vb-fghm87uJ-PRNOuUKjPNJCyRC5Cs0Ry0UBWvK8a4xKpSIMsScMlYCwKZFlKhEgKlLosGdS3ftBiS65_ZDiwujGu9i_gVO0ghLCbPT4vR7qdkpRDlP-zs5S97dcD2CDb2wdu0PxgOwW9UF3P1</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>Physical and cognitive doping in university students using the unrelated question model</title><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><creator>Dietz, Pavel ; Quermann, Anne ; van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria ; Striegel, Heiko ; Schröter, Hannes ; Ulrich, Rolf ; Simon, Perikles</creator><creatorcontrib>Dietz, Pavel ; Quermann, Anne ; van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria ; Striegel, Heiko ; Schröter, Hannes ; Ulrich, Rolf ; Simon, Perikles</creatorcontrib><description>Study objectives A short paper-and-pencil questionnaire was distributed to 1.243 university students assessing the 12-month prevalence of physical and cognitive doping using two versions of the UQM with different probabilities for receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3). Likelihood ratio tests were used to assess whether the prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping differed significantly between p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3. The order of questions (physical doping and cognitive doping) as well as the probability of receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 or p [almost equal to] 2/3) were counterbalanced across participants. Statistical power analyses were performed to determine sample size. The prevalence estimate for physical doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3 was 22.5% (95% CI: 10.8-34.1), and 12.8% (95% CI: 7.6-18.0) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. For cognitive doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3, the estimated prevalence was 22.5% (95% CI: 11.0-34.1), whereas it was 18.0% (95% CI: 12.5-23.5) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. Likelihood-ratio tests revealed that prevalence estimates for both physical and cognitive doping, respectively, did not differ significantly under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3 (physical doping: X.sup.2 = 2.25, df = 1, p = 0.13; cognitive doping: X.sup.2 = 0.49, df = 1, p = 0.48). Bayes factors computed with the Savage-Dickey method favored the null ("the prevalence estimates are identical under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") over the alternative ("the prevalence estimates differ under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") hypothesis for both physical doping (BF = 2.3) and cognitive doping (BF = 5.3). The present results suggest that prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping assessed by the UQM are largely unaffected by the probability for receiving the sensitive question p.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197270</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Analysis ; College students ; Management ; Physical fitness ; Surveys ; Universities and colleges</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2018-05, Vol.13 (5), p.e0197270</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2018 Public Library of Science</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,860,27901,27902</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Dietz, Pavel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Quermann, Anne</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Striegel, Heiko</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schröter, Hannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ulrich, Rolf</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Simon, Perikles</creatorcontrib><title>Physical and cognitive doping in university students using the unrelated question model</title><title>PloS one</title><description>Study objectives A short paper-and-pencil questionnaire was distributed to 1.243 university students assessing the 12-month prevalence of physical and cognitive doping using two versions of the UQM with different probabilities for receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3). Likelihood ratio tests were used to assess whether the prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping differed significantly between p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3. The order of questions (physical doping and cognitive doping) as well as the probability of receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 or p [almost equal to] 2/3) were counterbalanced across participants. Statistical power analyses were performed to determine sample size. The prevalence estimate for physical doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3 was 22.5% (95% CI: 10.8-34.1), and 12.8% (95% CI: 7.6-18.0) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. For cognitive doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3, the estimated prevalence was 22.5% (95% CI: 11.0-34.1), whereas it was 18.0% (95% CI: 12.5-23.5) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. Likelihood-ratio tests revealed that prevalence estimates for both physical and cognitive doping, respectively, did not differ significantly under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3 (physical doping: X.sup.2 = 2.25, df = 1, p = 0.13; cognitive doping: X.sup.2 = 0.49, df = 1, p = 0.48). Bayes factors computed with the Savage-Dickey method favored the null ("the prevalence estimates are identical under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") over the alternative ("the prevalence estimates differ under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") hypothesis for both physical doping (BF = 2.3) and cognitive doping (BF = 5.3). The present results suggest that prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping assessed by the UQM are largely unaffected by the probability for receiving the sensitive question p.</description><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>College students</subject><subject>Management</subject><subject>Physical fitness</subject><subject>Surveys</subject><subject>Universities and colleges</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2018</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFj01LAzEYhIMoWKv_wENOgoddk81-JMdS_CgUKlr0WF43b3dTYlKbROy_t0UP9eRpZpiHgSHkkrOci4bfrHzaOLD52jvMGVdN0bAjMuBKFFldMHF84E_JWQgrxioh63pAXh_7bTAtWApO09Z3zkTziVT7tXEdNY4mt8ubYOKWhpg0uhhoCvsy9rhrN2ghoqYfCUM03tF3r9Gek5Ml2IAXvzok87vb-fghm87uJ-PRNOuUKjPNJCyRC5Cs0Ry0UBWvK8a4xKpSIMsScMlYCwKZFlKhEgKlLosGdS3ftBiS65_ZDiwujGu9i_gVO0ghLCbPT4vR7qdkpRDlP-zs5S97dcD2CDb2wdu0PxgOwW9UF3P1</recordid><startdate>20180515</startdate><enddate>20180515</enddate><creator>Dietz, Pavel</creator><creator>Quermann, Anne</creator><creator>van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria</creator><creator>Striegel, Heiko</creator><creator>Schröter, Hannes</creator><creator>Ulrich, Rolf</creator><creator>Simon, Perikles</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20180515</creationdate><title>Physical and cognitive doping in university students using the unrelated question model</title><author>Dietz, Pavel ; Quermann, Anne ; van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria ; Striegel, Heiko ; Schröter, Hannes ; Ulrich, Rolf ; Simon, Perikles</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-g994-d08afe13a807d1ad3951650018e559a844aef00ca3e0d389e933e8d427ed68bd3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2018</creationdate><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>College students</topic><topic>Management</topic><topic>Physical fitness</topic><topic>Surveys</topic><topic>Universities and colleges</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Dietz, Pavel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Quermann, Anne</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Striegel, Heiko</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schröter, Hannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ulrich, Rolf</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Simon, Perikles</creatorcontrib><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Dietz, Pavel</au><au>Quermann, Anne</au><au>van Poppel, Mireille Nicoline Maria</au><au>Striegel, Heiko</au><au>Schröter, Hannes</au><au>Ulrich, Rolf</au><au>Simon, Perikles</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Physical and cognitive doping in university students using the unrelated question model</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><date>2018-05-15</date><risdate>2018</risdate><volume>13</volume><issue>5</issue><spage>e0197270</spage><pages>e0197270-</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Study objectives A short paper-and-pencil questionnaire was distributed to 1.243 university students assessing the 12-month prevalence of physical and cognitive doping using two versions of the UQM with different probabilities for receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3). Likelihood ratio tests were used to assess whether the prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping differed significantly between p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3. The order of questions (physical doping and cognitive doping) as well as the probability of receiving the sensitive question (p [almost equal to] 1/3 or p [almost equal to] 2/3) were counterbalanced across participants. Statistical power analyses were performed to determine sample size. The prevalence estimate for physical doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3 was 22.5% (95% CI: 10.8-34.1), and 12.8% (95% CI: 7.6-18.0) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. For cognitive doping with p [almost equal to] 1/3, the estimated prevalence was 22.5% (95% CI: 11.0-34.1), whereas it was 18.0% (95% CI: 12.5-23.5) with p [almost equal to] 2/3. Likelihood-ratio tests revealed that prevalence estimates for both physical and cognitive doping, respectively, did not differ significantly under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3 (physical doping: X.sup.2 = 2.25, df = 1, p = 0.13; cognitive doping: X.sup.2 = 0.49, df = 1, p = 0.48). Bayes factors computed with the Savage-Dickey method favored the null ("the prevalence estimates are identical under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") over the alternative ("the prevalence estimates differ under p [almost equal to] 1/3 and p [almost equal to] 2/3") hypothesis for both physical doping (BF = 2.3) and cognitive doping (BF = 5.3). The present results suggest that prevalence estimates for physical and cognitive doping assessed by the UQM are largely unaffected by the probability for receiving the sensitive question p.</abstract><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0197270</doi><tpages>e0197270</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2018-05, Vol.13 (5), p.e0197270
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_gale_incontextgauss_ISR_A538804334
source DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry; Public Library of Science (PLoS)
subjects Analysis
College students
Management
Physical fitness
Surveys
Universities and colleges
title Physical and cognitive doping in university students using the unrelated question model
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-04T20%3A15%3A18IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Physical%20and%20cognitive%20doping%20in%20university%20students%20using%20the%20unrelated%20question%20model&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Dietz,%20Pavel&rft.date=2018-05-15&rft.volume=13&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=e0197270&rft.pages=e0197270-&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0197270&rft_dat=%3Cgale%3EA538804334%3C/gale%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_galeid=A538804334&rfr_iscdi=true