Sign Language Input to Community Residents with Mental Retardation

Since teaching sign language to persons with mental retardation has increased dramatically during the past two decades, one might question whether the social environment is adequately prepared to teach it. Do significant others have the needed sign language competence to be effective models for sign...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Education and training in mental retardation 1991-06, Vol.26 (2), p.207-213
Hauptverfasser: Bryen, Diane Nelson, McGinley, Vicki
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 213
container_issue 2
container_start_page 207
container_title Education and training in mental retardation
container_volume 26
creator Bryen, Diane Nelson
McGinley, Vicki
description Since teaching sign language to persons with mental retardation has increased dramatically during the past two decades, one might question whether the social environment is adequately prepared to teach it. Do significant others have the needed sign language competence to be effective models for sign language learning? Do they use sign language when interacting with learners of sign? In order to answer these two research questions, 17 adults with mental retardation who were currently living in 10 community residential settings and their 11 habilitation instructors were questioned and observed. Results showed that residents had been learning sign for an average of 3.19 years, could imitate an average of 16.47 signs, could spontaneously produce an average of 20 one-word signs and 14.71 sign combinations. Staff's sign language competence was only slightly better than that of the residents (mean = 23 signs). Furthermore, staff's use of sign during interactions with residents was dismally limited—an average of less than two signs per 30-minute observational period. In fact, during the large majority of time, staff failed to interact with residents at all. When staff did use sign, they did so when teaching the sign (2.4%), when interacting socially with the resident (2.9%), and when interacting with someone else in the presence of the resident (0.6%). The implications of these findings for sign language learning are discussed, as well as the need for and nature of staff training.
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>jstor_eric_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_eric_primary_EJ431346</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ431346</ericid><jstor_id>23878590</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>23878590</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-e430-b7552f195cb95e0bfff9f9f7380e43217b7c877e6721e77d9f67b4e68977e89f3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNo9j1FLwzAcxPOg4Jx-A4V8gULStPknj1qmTirCtveRtEnNWNORpMi-vYHJuIc77gcHd4MWlFRlIUUt79B9jAdCGJFcLNDr1g0et8oPsxoMXvvTnHCacDON4-xdOuONia43PkX869IP_spRHXObVOhVcpN_QLdWHaN5_Pcl2r2tds1H0X6_r5uXtjAVI4WGui4tlXWnZW2IttbKLGCCZF5S0NAJAMOhpAagl5aDrgwXMpdCWrZET5dZE1y3PwU3qnDerz4rRlnFM36-4ENMU7jykgnIrwn7A2JHS38</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Index Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>Sign Language Input to Community Residents with Mental Retardation</title><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><creator>Bryen, Diane Nelson ; McGinley, Vicki</creator><creatorcontrib>Bryen, Diane Nelson ; McGinley, Vicki</creatorcontrib><description>Since teaching sign language to persons with mental retardation has increased dramatically during the past two decades, one might question whether the social environment is adequately prepared to teach it. Do significant others have the needed sign language competence to be effective models for sign language learning? Do they use sign language when interacting with learners of sign? In order to answer these two research questions, 17 adults with mental retardation who were currently living in 10 community residential settings and their 11 habilitation instructors were questioned and observed. Results showed that residents had been learning sign for an average of 3.19 years, could imitate an average of 16.47 signs, could spontaneously produce an average of 20 one-word signs and 14.71 sign combinations. Staff's sign language competence was only slightly better than that of the residents (mean = 23 signs). Furthermore, staff's use of sign during interactions with residents was dismally limited—an average of less than two signs per 30-minute observational period. In fact, during the large majority of time, staff failed to interact with residents at all. When staff did use sign, they did so when teaching the sign (2.4%), when interacting socially with the resident (2.9%), and when interacting with someone else in the presence of the resident (0.6%). The implications of these findings for sign language learning are discussed, as well as the need for and nature of staff training.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1042-9859</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>The Council for Exceptional Children, Division on Mental Retardation</publisher><subject>Adults ; Communication Skills ; Community power ; Competence ; Employee interaction ; Group Homes ; Intellectual disability ; Interaction ; Interpersonal Communication ; Language ; Mental Retardation ; Retirement communities ; Sign Language ; Sign languages ; Social interaction ; Social interaction models ; Special education ; Staff Development ; Teachers ; Trainers</subject><ispartof>Education and training in mental retardation, 1991-06, Vol.26 (2), p.207-213</ispartof><rights>Copyright 1991 Division on Mental Retardation, The Council for Exceptional Children</rights><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/23878590$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/23878590$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,803,58017,58250</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ431346$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Bryen, Diane Nelson</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>McGinley, Vicki</creatorcontrib><title>Sign Language Input to Community Residents with Mental Retardation</title><title>Education and training in mental retardation</title><description>Since teaching sign language to persons with mental retardation has increased dramatically during the past two decades, one might question whether the social environment is adequately prepared to teach it. Do significant others have the needed sign language competence to be effective models for sign language learning? Do they use sign language when interacting with learners of sign? In order to answer these two research questions, 17 adults with mental retardation who were currently living in 10 community residential settings and their 11 habilitation instructors were questioned and observed. Results showed that residents had been learning sign for an average of 3.19 years, could imitate an average of 16.47 signs, could spontaneously produce an average of 20 one-word signs and 14.71 sign combinations. Staff's sign language competence was only slightly better than that of the residents (mean = 23 signs). Furthermore, staff's use of sign during interactions with residents was dismally limited—an average of less than two signs per 30-minute observational period. In fact, during the large majority of time, staff failed to interact with residents at all. When staff did use sign, they did so when teaching the sign (2.4%), when interacting socially with the resident (2.9%), and when interacting with someone else in the presence of the resident (0.6%). The implications of these findings for sign language learning are discussed, as well as the need for and nature of staff training.</description><subject>Adults</subject><subject>Communication Skills</subject><subject>Community power</subject><subject>Competence</subject><subject>Employee interaction</subject><subject>Group Homes</subject><subject>Intellectual disability</subject><subject>Interaction</subject><subject>Interpersonal Communication</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Mental Retardation</subject><subject>Retirement communities</subject><subject>Sign Language</subject><subject>Sign languages</subject><subject>Social interaction</subject><subject>Social interaction models</subject><subject>Special education</subject><subject>Staff Development</subject><subject>Teachers</subject><subject>Trainers</subject><issn>1042-9859</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1991</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNo9j1FLwzAcxPOg4Jx-A4V8gULStPknj1qmTirCtveRtEnNWNORpMi-vYHJuIc77gcHd4MWlFRlIUUt79B9jAdCGJFcLNDr1g0et8oPsxoMXvvTnHCacDON4-xdOuONia43PkX869IP_spRHXObVOhVcpN_QLdWHaN5_Pcl2r2tds1H0X6_r5uXtjAVI4WGui4tlXWnZW2IttbKLGCCZF5S0NAJAMOhpAagl5aDrgwXMpdCWrZET5dZE1y3PwU3qnDerz4rRlnFM36-4ENMU7jykgnIrwn7A2JHS38</recordid><startdate>19910601</startdate><enddate>19910601</enddate><creator>Bryen, Diane Nelson</creator><creator>McGinley, Vicki</creator><general>The Council for Exceptional Children, Division on Mental Retardation</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19910601</creationdate><title>Sign Language Input to Community Residents with Mental Retardation</title><author>Bryen, Diane Nelson ; McGinley, Vicki</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-e430-b7552f195cb95e0bfff9f9f7380e43217b7c877e6721e77d9f67b4e68977e89f3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1991</creationdate><topic>Adults</topic><topic>Communication Skills</topic><topic>Community power</topic><topic>Competence</topic><topic>Employee interaction</topic><topic>Group Homes</topic><topic>Intellectual disability</topic><topic>Interaction</topic><topic>Interpersonal Communication</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Mental Retardation</topic><topic>Retirement communities</topic><topic>Sign Language</topic><topic>Sign languages</topic><topic>Social interaction</topic><topic>Social interaction models</topic><topic>Special education</topic><topic>Staff Development</topic><topic>Teachers</topic><topic>Trainers</topic><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Bryen, Diane Nelson</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>McGinley, Vicki</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><jtitle>Education and training in mental retardation</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Bryen, Diane Nelson</au><au>McGinley, Vicki</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ431346</ericid><atitle>Sign Language Input to Community Residents with Mental Retardation</atitle><jtitle>Education and training in mental retardation</jtitle><date>1991-06-01</date><risdate>1991</risdate><volume>26</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>207</spage><epage>213</epage><pages>207-213</pages><issn>1042-9859</issn><abstract>Since teaching sign language to persons with mental retardation has increased dramatically during the past two decades, one might question whether the social environment is adequately prepared to teach it. Do significant others have the needed sign language competence to be effective models for sign language learning? Do they use sign language when interacting with learners of sign? In order to answer these two research questions, 17 adults with mental retardation who were currently living in 10 community residential settings and their 11 habilitation instructors were questioned and observed. Results showed that residents had been learning sign for an average of 3.19 years, could imitate an average of 16.47 signs, could spontaneously produce an average of 20 one-word signs and 14.71 sign combinations. Staff's sign language competence was only slightly better than that of the residents (mean = 23 signs). Furthermore, staff's use of sign during interactions with residents was dismally limited—an average of less than two signs per 30-minute observational period. In fact, during the large majority of time, staff failed to interact with residents at all. When staff did use sign, they did so when teaching the sign (2.4%), when interacting socially with the resident (2.9%), and when interacting with someone else in the presence of the resident (0.6%). The implications of these findings for sign language learning are discussed, as well as the need for and nature of staff training.</abstract><pub>The Council for Exceptional Children, Division on Mental Retardation</pub><tpages>7</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1042-9859
ispartof Education and training in mental retardation, 1991-06, Vol.26 (2), p.207-213
issn 1042-9859
language eng
recordid cdi_eric_primary_EJ431346
source JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing
subjects Adults
Communication Skills
Community power
Competence
Employee interaction
Group Homes
Intellectual disability
Interaction
Interpersonal Communication
Language
Mental Retardation
Retirement communities
Sign Language
Sign languages
Social interaction
Social interaction models
Special education
Staff Development
Teachers
Trainers
title Sign Language Input to Community Residents with Mental Retardation
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-26T03%3A44%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_eric_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sign%20Language%20Input%20to%20Community%20Residents%20with%20Mental%20Retardation&rft.jtitle=Education%20and%20training%20in%20mental%20retardation&rft.au=Bryen,%20Diane%20Nelson&rft.date=1991-06-01&rft.volume=26&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=207&rft.epage=213&rft.pages=207-213&rft.issn=1042-9859&rft_id=info:doi/&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_eric_%3E23878590%3C/jstor_eric_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_ericid=EJ431346&rft_jstor_id=23878590&rfr_iscdi=true