Disfuencies Signal Reference to Novel Objects for Adults but Not Children

Speech disfluencies can guide the ways in which listeners interpret spoken language. Here, we examined whether three-year-olds, five-year-olds, and adults use filled pauses to anticipate that a speaker is likely to refer to a novel object. Across three experiments, participants were presented with p...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of child language 2018-05, Vol.45 (3), p.581
Hauptverfasser: Owens, Sarah J, Thacker, Justine M, Graham, Susan A
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page
container_issue 3
container_start_page 581
container_title Journal of child language
container_volume 45
creator Owens, Sarah J
Thacker, Justine M
Graham, Susan A
description Speech disfluencies can guide the ways in which listeners interpret spoken language. Here, we examined whether three-year-olds, five-year-olds, and adults use filled pauses to anticipate that a speaker is likely to refer to a novel object. Across three experiments, participants were presented with pairs of novel and familiar objects and heard a speaker refer to one of the objects using a fluent ("Look at the ball/lep!") or disfluent ("Look at thee uh ball/lep!") expression. The salience of the speaker's unfamiliarity with the novel referents, and the way in which the speaker referred to the novel referents (i.e., a noun vs. a description) varied across experiments. Three- and five-year-olds successfully identified familiar and novel targets, but only adults' looking patterns reflected increased looks to novel objects in the presence of a disfluency. Together, these findings demonstrate that adults, but not young children, use filled pauses to anticipate reference to novel objects.
doi_str_mv 10.1017/S0305000917000368
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>eric</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_eric_primary_EJ1177292</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ1177292</ericid><sourcerecordid>EJ1177292</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-eric_primary_EJ11772923</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFissKwjAURLNQsD4-wIWQH6jetGjapdSKulCw7ksft5oSW0lSwb83BfduZoZzhpA5gyUDxlcJ-LAGgJBxm_4mGBCnR27PRmSsdd0vCAOHHHdCVx02hUBNE3FvMkmvWKGyCKlp6bl9o6SXvMbCaFq1im7LTtqZd8ZKQ6OHkKW9T8mwyqTG2a8nZLGPb9HBRSWK9KXEM1OfND4xxrkXev4__wXN0TuP</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Index Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>Disfuencies Signal Reference to Novel Objects for Adults but Not Children</title><source>Cambridge University Press Journals Complete</source><creator>Owens, Sarah J ; Thacker, Justine M ; Graham, Susan A</creator><creatorcontrib>Owens, Sarah J ; Thacker, Justine M ; Graham, Susan A</creatorcontrib><description>Speech disfluencies can guide the ways in which listeners interpret spoken language. Here, we examined whether three-year-olds, five-year-olds, and adults use filled pauses to anticipate that a speaker is likely to refer to a novel object. Across three experiments, participants were presented with pairs of novel and familiar objects and heard a speaker refer to one of the objects using a fluent ("Look at the ball/lep!") or disfluent ("Look at thee uh ball/lep!") expression. The salience of the speaker's unfamiliarity with the novel referents, and the way in which the speaker referred to the novel referents (i.e., a noun vs. a description) varied across experiments. Three- and five-year-olds successfully identified familiar and novel targets, but only adults' looking patterns reflected increased looks to novel objects in the presence of a disfluency. Together, these findings demonstrate that adults, but not young children, use filled pauses to anticipate reference to novel objects.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0305-0009</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S0305000917000368</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>Adults ; Age Differences ; Familiarity ; Language Fluency ; Language Usage ; Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) ; Speech Communication ; Young Children</subject><ispartof>Journal of child language, 2018-05, Vol.45 (3), p.581</ispartof><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1177292$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Owens, Sarah J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Thacker, Justine M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Graham, Susan A</creatorcontrib><title>Disfuencies Signal Reference to Novel Objects for Adults but Not Children</title><title>Journal of child language</title><description>Speech disfluencies can guide the ways in which listeners interpret spoken language. Here, we examined whether three-year-olds, five-year-olds, and adults use filled pauses to anticipate that a speaker is likely to refer to a novel object. Across three experiments, participants were presented with pairs of novel and familiar objects and heard a speaker refer to one of the objects using a fluent ("Look at the ball/lep!") or disfluent ("Look at thee uh ball/lep!") expression. The salience of the speaker's unfamiliarity with the novel referents, and the way in which the speaker referred to the novel referents (i.e., a noun vs. a description) varied across experiments. Three- and five-year-olds successfully identified familiar and novel targets, but only adults' looking patterns reflected increased looks to novel objects in the presence of a disfluency. Together, these findings demonstrate that adults, but not young children, use filled pauses to anticipate reference to novel objects.</description><subject>Adults</subject><subject>Age Differences</subject><subject>Familiarity</subject><subject>Language Fluency</subject><subject>Language Usage</subject><subject>Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)</subject><subject>Speech Communication</subject><subject>Young Children</subject><issn>0305-0009</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2018</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFissKwjAURLNQsD4-wIWQH6jetGjapdSKulCw7ksft5oSW0lSwb83BfduZoZzhpA5gyUDxlcJ-LAGgJBxm_4mGBCnR27PRmSsdd0vCAOHHHdCVx02hUBNE3FvMkmvWKGyCKlp6bl9o6SXvMbCaFq1im7LTtqZd8ZKQ6OHkKW9T8mwyqTG2a8nZLGPb9HBRSWK9KXEM1OfND4xxrkXev4__wXN0TuP</recordid><startdate>201805</startdate><enddate>201805</enddate><creator>Owens, Sarah J</creator><creator>Thacker, Justine M</creator><creator>Graham, Susan A</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201805</creationdate><title>Disfuencies Signal Reference to Novel Objects for Adults but Not Children</title><author>Owens, Sarah J ; Thacker, Justine M ; Graham, Susan A</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-eric_primary_EJ11772923</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2018</creationdate><topic>Adults</topic><topic>Age Differences</topic><topic>Familiarity</topic><topic>Language Fluency</topic><topic>Language Usage</topic><topic>Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)</topic><topic>Speech Communication</topic><topic>Young Children</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Owens, Sarah J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Thacker, Justine M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Graham, Susan A</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><jtitle>Journal of child language</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Owens, Sarah J</au><au>Thacker, Justine M</au><au>Graham, Susan A</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1177292</ericid><atitle>Disfuencies Signal Reference to Novel Objects for Adults but Not Children</atitle><jtitle>Journal of child language</jtitle><date>2018-05</date><risdate>2018</risdate><volume>45</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>581</spage><pages>581-</pages><issn>0305-0009</issn><abstract>Speech disfluencies can guide the ways in which listeners interpret spoken language. Here, we examined whether three-year-olds, five-year-olds, and adults use filled pauses to anticipate that a speaker is likely to refer to a novel object. Across three experiments, participants were presented with pairs of novel and familiar objects and heard a speaker refer to one of the objects using a fluent ("Look at the ball/lep!") or disfluent ("Look at thee uh ball/lep!") expression. The salience of the speaker's unfamiliarity with the novel referents, and the way in which the speaker referred to the novel referents (i.e., a noun vs. a description) varied across experiments. Three- and five-year-olds successfully identified familiar and novel targets, but only adults' looking patterns reflected increased looks to novel objects in the presence of a disfluency. Together, these findings demonstrate that adults, but not young children, use filled pauses to anticipate reference to novel objects.</abstract><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><doi>10.1017/S0305000917000368</doi><tpages>29</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0305-0009
ispartof Journal of child language, 2018-05, Vol.45 (3), p.581
issn 0305-0009
language eng
recordid cdi_eric_primary_EJ1177292
source Cambridge University Press Journals Complete
subjects Adults
Age Differences
Familiarity
Language Fluency
Language Usage
Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Speech Communication
Young Children
title Disfuencies Signal Reference to Novel Objects for Adults but Not Children
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-27T12%3A05%3A17IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-eric&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Disfuencies%20Signal%20Reference%20to%20Novel%20Objects%20for%20Adults%20but%20Not%20Children&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20child%20language&rft.au=Owens,%20Sarah%20J&rft.date=2018-05&rft.volume=45&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=581&rft.pages=581-&rft.issn=0305-0009&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S0305000917000368&rft_dat=%3Ceric%3EEJ1177292%3C/eric%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_ericid=EJ1177292&rfr_iscdi=true