THE EFFECTS OF SIDE-PRESSURE ON PRESSURE SENSATION

Pressure and touch are often described as the same kind of sensation because they are elicited by similar mechanical stimulation. But they are quite different subjectively as well as electro-physiologically. The author discriminated between them by the difference of subjective sensation, and studied...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Shinrigaku kenkyū 1959/01/15, Vol.29(5), pp.303-312
1. Verfasser: YAI, HIDEKO
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng ; jpn
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 312
container_issue 5
container_start_page 303
container_title Shinrigaku kenkyū
container_volume 29
creator YAI, HIDEKO
description Pressure and touch are often described as the same kind of sensation because they are elicited by similar mechanical stimulation. But they are quite different subjectively as well as electro-physiologically. The author discriminated between them by the difference of subjective sensation, and studied the effects of additional side-pressure applied to relatively broad surface of the body upon pressure sensation. The sensitivity of pressure sensation was measured by the number of pressure spots within a given area of the skin. In normal resting subjects, the localization of pressure spots changes from one examination to another (Fig. 1), but the number is always nearly constant (Table 1). The author studied how side-pressure by a weight given to various parts of the body surface influenced upon the pressure sensitivity in the dorsal side of right forearm, and obtained the following results: 1) Side-pressure caused the decrease of pressure sensitivity in general. 2) The nearer was the location of side-pressure to the test area, the greater was it's decreasing effect (Table 2). 3) However, the decreasing effect of side-pressure applied to the dorsal side of right hand (distal from the test area) was less than that to the dorsal side of right forearm (proximal from the test area), even if both distances from the test area were nearly equal. This difference of the effect may be explained by the dermatome: the skin of the hand was innervated by the spinal nerve differing from that of the test area, whereas the forearm was innervated by the same nerve. But there is a problem on such simplified explanation. 4) The effect increased as the size and intensity of side-pressure increased. Side-pressure given on the forearm by a cuff also depressed the pressure sensitivity. In this procedure, the asphyxia took place and the sensation subjectively differed from the former case. So, in the latter case another mechanism might be considered. 5) The effect of side-pressure showed the maximum value within 1min. after pressure application (Fig. 2), regardless of intensity and location of applied pressure. Consequently, it may not be considered that the effect of side-pressure spreads around from the pressed area. 6) The after-effect was recognized and it disappeared within 1min. (Fig. 3). The obtained results can not be simply explained by the pressure block of the peripheral nerve trunks. The sensitivity of pressure sensation is depressed also by the other stimulation of the ce
doi_str_mv 10.4992/jjpsy.29.303
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>jstage_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_crossref_primary_10_4992_jjpsy_29_303</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>article_jjpsy1926_29_5_29_5_303_article_char_en</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3073-f169c3194cb43b8d8935b2dfc9c16fb1e07988e1175b74e568e2750d4f116c2e3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNo9j8tugzAQRa2qlYrS7PoBfEChHj_AXkbUNEgRVIGsLTCmBaVthLPJ35eEis3MSPfckQ5Cz4BDJiV5HYaTu4REhhTTO-SBECwALMg98jAmEHBCo0e0dq5vMMEs5gyYh0i1Vb5KU5VUpV-kfpm9qeBjr8rysFd-kfvLXaq83FRZkT-hh64-Orv-3yt0SFWVbINd8Z4lm11gKI5p0EEkDQXJTMNoI1ohKW9I2xlpIOoasDiWQliAmDcxszwSlsQct6wDiAyxdIVe5r9m_HVutJ0-jf13PV40YH1V1jdlTaSelCdczfjgzvWnXeB6PPfmaGcYJImuBT6Pqbfk5qsetf2hf8CdXWg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>THE EFFECTS OF SIDE-PRESSURE ON PRESSURE SENSATION</title><source>J-STAGE (Japan Science &amp; Technology Information Aggregator, Electronic) Freely Available Titles - Japanese</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><creator>YAI, HIDEKO</creator><creatorcontrib>YAI, HIDEKO</creatorcontrib><description>Pressure and touch are often described as the same kind of sensation because they are elicited by similar mechanical stimulation. But they are quite different subjectively as well as electro-physiologically. The author discriminated between them by the difference of subjective sensation, and studied the effects of additional side-pressure applied to relatively broad surface of the body upon pressure sensation. The sensitivity of pressure sensation was measured by the number of pressure spots within a given area of the skin. In normal resting subjects, the localization of pressure spots changes from one examination to another (Fig. 1), but the number is always nearly constant (Table 1). The author studied how side-pressure by a weight given to various parts of the body surface influenced upon the pressure sensitivity in the dorsal side of right forearm, and obtained the following results: 1) Side-pressure caused the decrease of pressure sensitivity in general. 2) The nearer was the location of side-pressure to the test area, the greater was it's decreasing effect (Table 2). 3) However, the decreasing effect of side-pressure applied to the dorsal side of right hand (distal from the test area) was less than that to the dorsal side of right forearm (proximal from the test area), even if both distances from the test area were nearly equal. This difference of the effect may be explained by the dermatome: the skin of the hand was innervated by the spinal nerve differing from that of the test area, whereas the forearm was innervated by the same nerve. But there is a problem on such simplified explanation. 4) The effect increased as the size and intensity of side-pressure increased. Side-pressure given on the forearm by a cuff also depressed the pressure sensitivity. In this procedure, the asphyxia took place and the sensation subjectively differed from the former case. So, in the latter case another mechanism might be considered. 5) The effect of side-pressure showed the maximum value within 1min. after pressure application (Fig. 2), regardless of intensity and location of applied pressure. Consequently, it may not be considered that the effect of side-pressure spreads around from the pressed area. 6) The after-effect was recognized and it disappeared within 1min. (Fig. 3). The obtained results can not be simply explained by the pressure block of the peripheral nerve trunks. The sensitivity of pressure sensation is depressed also by the other stimulation of the central nervous system, especially the effects of stimuli, depressing the clarity of consciousness, are very similar to side-pressure's. It may be followed that side-pressure on the skin causes the inhibitory effect on the central nervous system including the cerebral cortex. It is concluded that the depression of pressure sensitivity is not of peripheral origin, such as pressure nerve block, but results from the induction in a level or levels of the central nervous system.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0021-5236</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1884-1082</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.29.303</identifier><language>eng ; jpn</language><publisher>The Japanese Psychological Association</publisher><ispartof>The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 1959/01/15, Vol.29(5), pp.303-312</ispartof><rights>The Japanese Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3073-f169c3194cb43b8d8935b2dfc9c16fb1e07988e1175b74e568e2750d4f116c2e3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,1883,4024,27923,27924,27925</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>YAI, HIDEKO</creatorcontrib><title>THE EFFECTS OF SIDE-PRESSURE ON PRESSURE SENSATION</title><title>Shinrigaku kenkyū</title><addtitle>The Japanese Journal of Psychology</addtitle><description>Pressure and touch are often described as the same kind of sensation because they are elicited by similar mechanical stimulation. But they are quite different subjectively as well as electro-physiologically. The author discriminated between them by the difference of subjective sensation, and studied the effects of additional side-pressure applied to relatively broad surface of the body upon pressure sensation. The sensitivity of pressure sensation was measured by the number of pressure spots within a given area of the skin. In normal resting subjects, the localization of pressure spots changes from one examination to another (Fig. 1), but the number is always nearly constant (Table 1). The author studied how side-pressure by a weight given to various parts of the body surface influenced upon the pressure sensitivity in the dorsal side of right forearm, and obtained the following results: 1) Side-pressure caused the decrease of pressure sensitivity in general. 2) The nearer was the location of side-pressure to the test area, the greater was it's decreasing effect (Table 2). 3) However, the decreasing effect of side-pressure applied to the dorsal side of right hand (distal from the test area) was less than that to the dorsal side of right forearm (proximal from the test area), even if both distances from the test area were nearly equal. This difference of the effect may be explained by the dermatome: the skin of the hand was innervated by the spinal nerve differing from that of the test area, whereas the forearm was innervated by the same nerve. But there is a problem on such simplified explanation. 4) The effect increased as the size and intensity of side-pressure increased. Side-pressure given on the forearm by a cuff also depressed the pressure sensitivity. In this procedure, the asphyxia took place and the sensation subjectively differed from the former case. So, in the latter case another mechanism might be considered. 5) The effect of side-pressure showed the maximum value within 1min. after pressure application (Fig. 2), regardless of intensity and location of applied pressure. Consequently, it may not be considered that the effect of side-pressure spreads around from the pressed area. 6) The after-effect was recognized and it disappeared within 1min. (Fig. 3). The obtained results can not be simply explained by the pressure block of the peripheral nerve trunks. The sensitivity of pressure sensation is depressed also by the other stimulation of the central nervous system, especially the effects of stimuli, depressing the clarity of consciousness, are very similar to side-pressure's. It may be followed that side-pressure on the skin causes the inhibitory effect on the central nervous system including the cerebral cortex. It is concluded that the depression of pressure sensitivity is not of peripheral origin, such as pressure nerve block, but results from the induction in a level or levels of the central nervous system.</description><issn>0021-5236</issn><issn>1884-1082</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1959</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNo9j8tugzAQRa2qlYrS7PoBfEChHj_AXkbUNEgRVIGsLTCmBaVthLPJ35eEis3MSPfckQ5Cz4BDJiV5HYaTu4REhhTTO-SBECwALMg98jAmEHBCo0e0dq5vMMEs5gyYh0i1Vb5KU5VUpV-kfpm9qeBjr8rysFd-kfvLXaq83FRZkT-hh64-Orv-3yt0SFWVbINd8Z4lm11gKI5p0EEkDQXJTMNoI1ohKW9I2xlpIOoasDiWQliAmDcxszwSlsQct6wDiAyxdIVe5r9m_HVutJ0-jf13PV40YH1V1jdlTaSelCdczfjgzvWnXeB6PPfmaGcYJImuBT6Pqbfk5qsetf2hf8CdXWg</recordid><startdate>1959</startdate><enddate>1959</enddate><creator>YAI, HIDEKO</creator><general>The Japanese Psychological Association</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope></search><sort><creationdate>1959</creationdate><title>THE EFFECTS OF SIDE-PRESSURE ON PRESSURE SENSATION</title><author>YAI, HIDEKO</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c3073-f169c3194cb43b8d8935b2dfc9c16fb1e07988e1175b74e568e2750d4f116c2e3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng ; jpn</language><creationdate>1959</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>YAI, HIDEKO</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><jtitle>Shinrigaku kenkyū</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>YAI, HIDEKO</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>THE EFFECTS OF SIDE-PRESSURE ON PRESSURE SENSATION</atitle><jtitle>Shinrigaku kenkyū</jtitle><addtitle>The Japanese Journal of Psychology</addtitle><date>1959</date><risdate>1959</risdate><volume>29</volume><issue>5</issue><spage>303</spage><epage>312</epage><pages>303-312</pages><issn>0021-5236</issn><eissn>1884-1082</eissn><abstract>Pressure and touch are often described as the same kind of sensation because they are elicited by similar mechanical stimulation. But they are quite different subjectively as well as electro-physiologically. The author discriminated between them by the difference of subjective sensation, and studied the effects of additional side-pressure applied to relatively broad surface of the body upon pressure sensation. The sensitivity of pressure sensation was measured by the number of pressure spots within a given area of the skin. In normal resting subjects, the localization of pressure spots changes from one examination to another (Fig. 1), but the number is always nearly constant (Table 1). The author studied how side-pressure by a weight given to various parts of the body surface influenced upon the pressure sensitivity in the dorsal side of right forearm, and obtained the following results: 1) Side-pressure caused the decrease of pressure sensitivity in general. 2) The nearer was the location of side-pressure to the test area, the greater was it's decreasing effect (Table 2). 3) However, the decreasing effect of side-pressure applied to the dorsal side of right hand (distal from the test area) was less than that to the dorsal side of right forearm (proximal from the test area), even if both distances from the test area were nearly equal. This difference of the effect may be explained by the dermatome: the skin of the hand was innervated by the spinal nerve differing from that of the test area, whereas the forearm was innervated by the same nerve. But there is a problem on such simplified explanation. 4) The effect increased as the size and intensity of side-pressure increased. Side-pressure given on the forearm by a cuff also depressed the pressure sensitivity. In this procedure, the asphyxia took place and the sensation subjectively differed from the former case. So, in the latter case another mechanism might be considered. 5) The effect of side-pressure showed the maximum value within 1min. after pressure application (Fig. 2), regardless of intensity and location of applied pressure. Consequently, it may not be considered that the effect of side-pressure spreads around from the pressed area. 6) The after-effect was recognized and it disappeared within 1min. (Fig. 3). The obtained results can not be simply explained by the pressure block of the peripheral nerve trunks. The sensitivity of pressure sensation is depressed also by the other stimulation of the central nervous system, especially the effects of stimuli, depressing the clarity of consciousness, are very similar to side-pressure's. It may be followed that side-pressure on the skin causes the inhibitory effect on the central nervous system including the cerebral cortex. It is concluded that the depression of pressure sensitivity is not of peripheral origin, such as pressure nerve block, but results from the induction in a level or levels of the central nervous system.</abstract><pub>The Japanese Psychological Association</pub><doi>10.4992/jjpsy.29.303</doi><tpages>10</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0021-5236
ispartof The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 1959/01/15, Vol.29(5), pp.303-312
issn 0021-5236
1884-1082
language eng ; jpn
recordid cdi_crossref_primary_10_4992_jjpsy_29_303
source J-STAGE (Japan Science & Technology Information Aggregator, Electronic) Freely Available Titles - Japanese; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals
title THE EFFECTS OF SIDE-PRESSURE ON PRESSURE SENSATION
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-26T23%3A18%3A26IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstage_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=THE%20EFFECTS%20OF%20SIDE-PRESSURE%20ON%20PRESSURE%20SENSATION&rft.jtitle=Shinrigaku%20kenky%C5%AB&rft.au=YAI,%20HIDEKO&rft.date=1959&rft.volume=29&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=303&rft.epage=312&rft.pages=303-312&rft.issn=0021-5236&rft.eissn=1884-1082&rft_id=info:doi/10.4992/jjpsy.29.303&rft_dat=%3Cjstage_cross%3Earticle_jjpsy1926_29_5_29_5_303_article_char_en%3C/jstage_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true