Angle of Camera View Influences Resumption Lag in a Visual-Motor Task

Prior research on interruptions examined the effects of different characteristics of the primary and interrupting tasks on performance of the primary task. One measure is the resumption lag– the time between the end of the interrupting task and the next action in the resumed primary task (Altmann &a...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 2017-09, Vol.61 (1), p.1291-1291
Hauptverfasser: Lodinger, Natalie R., DeLucia, Patricia R.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 1291
container_issue 1
container_start_page 1291
container_title Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting
container_volume 61
creator Lodinger, Natalie R.
DeLucia, Patricia R.
description Prior research on interruptions examined the effects of different characteristics of the primary and interrupting tasks on performance of the primary task. One measure is the resumption lag– the time between the end of the interrupting task and the next action in the resumed primary task (Altmann & Trafton, 2004). Prior research showed that an increase in the workload of a task results in an increase in resumption lag (Iqbal & Bailey, 2005). A common feature of prior studies of resumption lag is the use of computer-based tasks. However, interruptions occur in other types of tasks, such as laparoscopic surgery in which errors can result in serious consequences for the patient (Gillespie Chaboyer & Fairweather, 2012). Common interruptions during laparoscopic surgery include equipment failures and communication with team members (e.g., Gillespie et al.,2012). In laparoscopic surgery, a small incision is made in the patient, and a laparoscope is placed inside the body cavity. The surgeon typically views the surgical site on a two-dimensional screen rather than in three-dimensions as in open surgery (Chan et al., 1997). The two-dimensional camera image imposes perceptual and cognitive demands on the surgeon, such as impaired depth perception (Chan et al., 1997; DeLucia & Griswold, 2011) and a limited field-of-view of the site (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011). The present study examined whether top-view and side-view camera angles, which putatively impose different cognitive demands (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011), would differentially affect the resumption lag in a visual-motor task. Participants completed a peg transfer task in which they were interrupted with a mental rotation task of different durations and rotation angles. The duration of the mental rotation task was either short (6 s) or long (12 s), representing relatively low and high cognitive demands, respectively. Smaller rotation angles (0, 60, and 300 degrees from vertical) and greater rotation angles (120, 180 and 240 degrees from vertical) presumably imposed smaller and larger cognitive demands, respectively. Resumption lag was measured as the time between the end of the interruption and the first time a peg was touched in the resumed peg transfer task. Participants needed significantly more time to resume the peg transfer task with the side view compared to the top view, and with the longer mental rotation task duration compared to the shorter duration. The main effect of rotation angle was not significant.
doi_str_mv 10.1177/1541931213601803
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>sage_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_crossref_primary_10_1177_1541931213601803</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sage_id>10.1177_1541931213601803</sage_id><sourcerecordid>10.1177_1541931213601803</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c197t-61aed8b2b4be8b8ca2e9d37eb5b2376abb987e12a98189c51bb6492ee66e52d73</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kE1Lw0AYhBdRsFbvHvcPrO67m-zHsYSqhYog1WvYTd-U1GRTdhPEf29DexI8zWHmGYYh5B74A4DWj5BnYCUIkIqD4fKCzAQoy3Ku9CWZTTab_Gtyk9KecyG1zGZkuQi7Fmlf08J1GB39bPCbrkLdjhgqTPQd09gdhqYPdO12tAl0yqTRtey1H_pINy593ZKr2rUJ7846Jx9Py03xwtZvz6tisWYVWD0wBQ63xgufeTTeVE6g3UqNPvfHOcp5b41GEM4aMLbKwXuVWYGoFOZiq-Wc8FNvFfuUItblITadiz8l8HK6ofx7wxFhJyS5HZb7fozhuPD__C8Fylvd</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>Angle of Camera View Influences Resumption Lag in a Visual-Motor Task</title><source>Access via SAGE</source><creator>Lodinger, Natalie R. ; DeLucia, Patricia R.</creator><creatorcontrib>Lodinger, Natalie R. ; DeLucia, Patricia R.</creatorcontrib><description><![CDATA[Prior research on interruptions examined the effects of different characteristics of the primary and interrupting tasks on performance of the primary task. One measure is the resumption lag– the time between the end of the interrupting task and the next action in the resumed primary task (Altmann & Trafton, 2004). Prior research showed that an increase in the workload of a task results in an increase in resumption lag (Iqbal & Bailey, 2005). A common feature of prior studies of resumption lag is the use of computer-based tasks. However, interruptions occur in other types of tasks, such as laparoscopic surgery in which errors can result in serious consequences for the patient (Gillespie Chaboyer & Fairweather, 2012). Common interruptions during laparoscopic surgery include equipment failures and communication with team members (e.g., Gillespie et al.,2012). In laparoscopic surgery, a small incision is made in the patient, and a laparoscope is placed inside the body cavity. The surgeon typically views the surgical site on a two-dimensional screen rather than in three-dimensions as in open surgery (Chan et al., 1997). The two-dimensional camera image imposes perceptual and cognitive demands on the surgeon, such as impaired depth perception (Chan et al., 1997; DeLucia & Griswold, 2011) and a limited field-of-view of the site (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011). The present study examined whether top-view and side-view camera angles, which putatively impose different cognitive demands (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011), would differentially affect the resumption lag in a visual-motor task. Participants completed a peg transfer task in which they were interrupted with a mental rotation task of different durations and rotation angles. The duration of the mental rotation task was either short (6 s) or long (12 s), representing relatively low and high cognitive demands, respectively. Smaller rotation angles (0, 60, and 300 degrees from vertical) and greater rotation angles (120, 180 and 240 degrees from vertical) presumably imposed smaller and larger cognitive demands, respectively. Resumption lag was measured as the time between the end of the interruption and the first time a peg was touched in the resumed peg transfer task. Participants needed significantly more time to resume the peg transfer task with the side view compared to the top view, and with the longer mental rotation task duration compared to the shorter duration. The main effect of rotation angle was not significant. The side view also resulted in higher ratings of mental demand, effort, and frustration on the Raw Task Load Index (RTLX), the ratings-only portion of the NASA-TLX (Hart, 2006). Thus, a visual-motor task that is higher in cognitive demand can result in more time to resume a primary task following an interruption. Practical implications are that camera viewing angles associated with lower cognitive demands should be preferred in the operating room when feasible, and that interruption durations should be minimized. However, results also indicated that the side view resulted in longer movement times than the top view, even without an interruption, suggesting that factors other than cognitive demands may account for effects of camera angle on resumption lag; this should be examined in future research.]]></description><identifier>ISSN: 1541-9312</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 1071-1813</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2169-5067</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1177/1541931213601803</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications</publisher><ispartof>Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 2017-09, Vol.61 (1), p.1291-1291</ispartof><rights>2017 Human Factors and Ergonomics Society</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c197t-61aed8b2b4be8b8ca2e9d37eb5b2376abb987e12a98189c51bb6492ee66e52d73</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1541931213601803$$EPDF$$P50$$Gsage$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1541931213601803$$EHTML$$P50$$Gsage$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,21819,27924,27925,43621,43622</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Lodinger, Natalie R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>DeLucia, Patricia R.</creatorcontrib><title>Angle of Camera View Influences Resumption Lag in a Visual-Motor Task</title><title>Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting</title><description><![CDATA[Prior research on interruptions examined the effects of different characteristics of the primary and interrupting tasks on performance of the primary task. One measure is the resumption lag– the time between the end of the interrupting task and the next action in the resumed primary task (Altmann & Trafton, 2004). Prior research showed that an increase in the workload of a task results in an increase in resumption lag (Iqbal & Bailey, 2005). A common feature of prior studies of resumption lag is the use of computer-based tasks. However, interruptions occur in other types of tasks, such as laparoscopic surgery in which errors can result in serious consequences for the patient (Gillespie Chaboyer & Fairweather, 2012). Common interruptions during laparoscopic surgery include equipment failures and communication with team members (e.g., Gillespie et al.,2012). In laparoscopic surgery, a small incision is made in the patient, and a laparoscope is placed inside the body cavity. The surgeon typically views the surgical site on a two-dimensional screen rather than in three-dimensions as in open surgery (Chan et al., 1997). The two-dimensional camera image imposes perceptual and cognitive demands on the surgeon, such as impaired depth perception (Chan et al., 1997; DeLucia & Griswold, 2011) and a limited field-of-view of the site (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011). The present study examined whether top-view and side-view camera angles, which putatively impose different cognitive demands (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011), would differentially affect the resumption lag in a visual-motor task. Participants completed a peg transfer task in which they were interrupted with a mental rotation task of different durations and rotation angles. The duration of the mental rotation task was either short (6 s) or long (12 s), representing relatively low and high cognitive demands, respectively. Smaller rotation angles (0, 60, and 300 degrees from vertical) and greater rotation angles (120, 180 and 240 degrees from vertical) presumably imposed smaller and larger cognitive demands, respectively. Resumption lag was measured as the time between the end of the interruption and the first time a peg was touched in the resumed peg transfer task. Participants needed significantly more time to resume the peg transfer task with the side view compared to the top view, and with the longer mental rotation task duration compared to the shorter duration. The main effect of rotation angle was not significant. The side view also resulted in higher ratings of mental demand, effort, and frustration on the Raw Task Load Index (RTLX), the ratings-only portion of the NASA-TLX (Hart, 2006). Thus, a visual-motor task that is higher in cognitive demand can result in more time to resume a primary task following an interruption. Practical implications are that camera viewing angles associated with lower cognitive demands should be preferred in the operating room when feasible, and that interruption durations should be minimized. However, results also indicated that the side view resulted in longer movement times than the top view, even without an interruption, suggesting that factors other than cognitive demands may account for effects of camera angle on resumption lag; this should be examined in future research.]]></description><issn>1541-9312</issn><issn>1071-1813</issn><issn>2169-5067</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp1kE1Lw0AYhBdRsFbvHvcPrO67m-zHsYSqhYog1WvYTd-U1GRTdhPEf29DexI8zWHmGYYh5B74A4DWj5BnYCUIkIqD4fKCzAQoy3Ku9CWZTTab_Gtyk9KecyG1zGZkuQi7Fmlf08J1GB39bPCbrkLdjhgqTPQd09gdhqYPdO12tAl0yqTRtey1H_pINy593ZKr2rUJ7846Jx9Py03xwtZvz6tisWYVWD0wBQ63xgufeTTeVE6g3UqNPvfHOcp5b41GEM4aMLbKwXuVWYGoFOZiq-Wc8FNvFfuUItblITadiz8l8HK6ofx7wxFhJyS5HZb7fozhuPD__C8Fylvd</recordid><startdate>201709</startdate><enddate>201709</enddate><creator>Lodinger, Natalie R.</creator><creator>DeLucia, Patricia R.</creator><general>SAGE Publications</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201709</creationdate><title>Angle of Camera View Influences Resumption Lag in a Visual-Motor Task</title><author>Lodinger, Natalie R. ; DeLucia, Patricia R.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c197t-61aed8b2b4be8b8ca2e9d37eb5b2376abb987e12a98189c51bb6492ee66e52d73</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Lodinger, Natalie R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>DeLucia, Patricia R.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><jtitle>Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Lodinger, Natalie R.</au><au>DeLucia, Patricia R.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Angle of Camera View Influences Resumption Lag in a Visual-Motor Task</atitle><jtitle>Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting</jtitle><date>2017-09</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>61</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>1291</spage><epage>1291</epage><pages>1291-1291</pages><issn>1541-9312</issn><issn>1071-1813</issn><eissn>2169-5067</eissn><abstract><![CDATA[Prior research on interruptions examined the effects of different characteristics of the primary and interrupting tasks on performance of the primary task. One measure is the resumption lag– the time between the end of the interrupting task and the next action in the resumed primary task (Altmann & Trafton, 2004). Prior research showed that an increase in the workload of a task results in an increase in resumption lag (Iqbal & Bailey, 2005). A common feature of prior studies of resumption lag is the use of computer-based tasks. However, interruptions occur in other types of tasks, such as laparoscopic surgery in which errors can result in serious consequences for the patient (Gillespie Chaboyer & Fairweather, 2012). Common interruptions during laparoscopic surgery include equipment failures and communication with team members (e.g., Gillespie et al.,2012). In laparoscopic surgery, a small incision is made in the patient, and a laparoscope is placed inside the body cavity. The surgeon typically views the surgical site on a two-dimensional screen rather than in three-dimensions as in open surgery (Chan et al., 1997). The two-dimensional camera image imposes perceptual and cognitive demands on the surgeon, such as impaired depth perception (Chan et al., 1997; DeLucia & Griswold, 2011) and a limited field-of-view of the site (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011). The present study examined whether top-view and side-view camera angles, which putatively impose different cognitive demands (DeLucia & Griswold, 2011), would differentially affect the resumption lag in a visual-motor task. Participants completed a peg transfer task in which they were interrupted with a mental rotation task of different durations and rotation angles. The duration of the mental rotation task was either short (6 s) or long (12 s), representing relatively low and high cognitive demands, respectively. Smaller rotation angles (0, 60, and 300 degrees from vertical) and greater rotation angles (120, 180 and 240 degrees from vertical) presumably imposed smaller and larger cognitive demands, respectively. Resumption lag was measured as the time between the end of the interruption and the first time a peg was touched in the resumed peg transfer task. Participants needed significantly more time to resume the peg transfer task with the side view compared to the top view, and with the longer mental rotation task duration compared to the shorter duration. The main effect of rotation angle was not significant. The side view also resulted in higher ratings of mental demand, effort, and frustration on the Raw Task Load Index (RTLX), the ratings-only portion of the NASA-TLX (Hart, 2006). Thus, a visual-motor task that is higher in cognitive demand can result in more time to resume a primary task following an interruption. Practical implications are that camera viewing angles associated with lower cognitive demands should be preferred in the operating room when feasible, and that interruption durations should be minimized. However, results also indicated that the side view resulted in longer movement times than the top view, even without an interruption, suggesting that factors other than cognitive demands may account for effects of camera angle on resumption lag; this should be examined in future research.]]></abstract><cop>Los Angeles, CA</cop><pub>SAGE Publications</pub><doi>10.1177/1541931213601803</doi><tpages>1</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1541-9312
ispartof Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 2017-09, Vol.61 (1), p.1291-1291
issn 1541-9312
1071-1813
2169-5067
language eng
recordid cdi_crossref_primary_10_1177_1541931213601803
source Access via SAGE
title Angle of Camera View Influences Resumption Lag in a Visual-Motor Task
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-28T03%3A50%3A02IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-sage_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Angle%20of%20Camera%20View%20Influences%20Resumption%20Lag%20in%20a%20Visual-Motor%20Task&rft.jtitle=Proceedings%20of%20the%20Human%20Factors%20and%20Ergonomics%20Society%20Annual%20Meeting&rft.au=Lodinger,%20Natalie%20R.&rft.date=2017-09&rft.volume=61&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=1291&rft.epage=1291&rft.pages=1291-1291&rft.issn=1541-9312&rft.eissn=2169-5067&rft_id=info:doi/10.1177/1541931213601803&rft_dat=%3Csage_cross%3E10.1177_1541931213601803%3C/sage_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_sage_id=10.1177_1541931213601803&rfr_iscdi=true